Animal models of depression explained

Animal models of depression are research tools used to investigate depression and action of antidepressants. They are used as a simulation to investigate the symptomatology and pathophysiology of depressive illness and to screen novel antidepressants. These models provide insights into molecular, genetic, and epigenetic factors associated with depression. Criteria for valid animal models include face, construct, and predictive validity. Endophenotypes, such as anhedonia, behavioral despair, changes in appetite, neuroanatomical alterations, neuroendocrine disturbances, alterations in sleep architecture, and anxiety-related behaviors, are evaluated in these models. Antidepressant screening tests are employed to assess the effects of genetic, pharmacological, or environmental manipulations. Stress models including learned helplessness, chronic mild stress, and social defeat stress simulate the impact of stressors on depression. Early life stress models, psychostimulant withdrawal models, olfactory bulbectomy, and genetically engineered mice contribute to a comprehensive understanding of depression's etiology and potential therapeutic interventions.

Introduction

Depression

Major depressive disorder is commonly called "clinical depression" or "depression." It is a common, long-lasting and diverse psychiatric syndrome that significantly affects a person's thoughts, behavior, feelings and sense of well-being. According to the DSM-5, someone diagnosed with depression should be showing at least five symptoms and they should last two weeks.[1] Depression can include a variety of different symptoms and does not always look the same for everyone. Some of these symptoms may include sadness, anxiousness, emptiness, hopelessness, worry, helplessness, worthlessness, guilt, irritableness, hurt, or restlessness. People experiencing depression may also lose interest in activities that once were pleasurable, experience loss of appetite, overeat, have problems concentrating, remembering details, making decisions, and may contemplate or attempt suicide. Insomnia, excessive sleeping, fatigue, loss of energy, aches, pains, or digestive problems that are resistant to treatment may also be present.[2]

Modeling depression in animals

It is difficult to develop an animal model that perfectly reproduces the symptoms of depression in patients. It is generic that 3 standards may be used to evaluate the reliability of an animal version of depression: the phenomenological or morphological appearances (face validity), a comparable etiology (assemble validity), and healing similarities (predictive validity). Many animals lack self-consciousness, self-reflection, and consideration; moreover, hallmarks of the disorder such as depressed mood, low self-esteem or suicidality are hardly accessible in non-humans. However, depression, as other mental disorders, consists of endophenotypes[3] that can be reproduced independently and evaluated in animals. An ideal animal model offers an opportunity to understand molecular, genetic, and epigenetic factors that may lead to depression. By using animal models, the underlying molecular alterations and the causal relationship between genetic, or environmental, alterations and depression can be examined. This would afford a better insight into pathology of depression because animal models are indispensable for identifying novel therapies for depression.[4]

Endophenotypes in animal model of depression

The following endophenotypes have been described:

The loss of interest is a core symptom of depression. Anhedonia in rodents can be assessed by sucrose preference or by intracranial self-stimulation.

Behavioral despair might be assessed with tests such as the forced-swimming test or the tail suspension test.

Criteria for valid animal models of depression

An appropriate animal model of human depression should fulfill the following criteria as much as possible: strong phenomenological similarities and similar pathophysiology (face validity), comparable etiology (construct validity), and common treatment (predictive validity).[6] [7] [8] Depression is a heterogeneous disorder and its many symptoms are hard to produce in laboratory animals. When studying depression used in animals originally, symptoms equivalent to odd social behavior and emotion were used to determine if the animal had depression.[9] The question therefore remains whether we can know if the animal is "depressed". They are unable to have the emotions that are associated specifically with humans, like sadness.[10] Few models of depression fully fit these validating criteria, and most models currently used rely on either actions of known antidepressants or responses to stress. It is not necessary for an "ideal" animal model of depression to exhibit all the abnormalities of depression-relevant behaviors, just as not all patients manifest every possible symptom of depression.

Creating models

Research use a number of standardized ways to induce depression-like symptoms in lab animals. The most commonly used type of models are based on stress.

Stress models

Certain types of human depression are precipitated by stressful life events, and vulnerable individuals experiencing these stressors. Consequently, the majority of animal models of depression are based on the exposure to various types of acute or chronic stressors.

Adult stress models

Early life stress models

Early adverse experiences such as traumatic life events in childhood result in an increased sensitivity to the effects of stress later in life and influence the vulnerability to depression.[25] Suitable animal models could provide a basis for understanding potential mechanisms of environmental and developmental factors of individual differences in stress reactivity and vulnerability to disorders. Models of early life stress involve prenatal stress, early postnatal handling and maternal separation. All these treatments have been demonstrated to produce significant effects that last until adulthood.

Other models

Evaluating symptoms

The degree of depression-like symptoms in an animal is evaluated using a number of tests. Tests provide a measure of an animal's response to inescapable stress (lack of attempt to escape is seen as despair/hopelessness) and to reward (lack of response indicates anhedonia), or to measure its degree of anxiety.

Despair-based

[33] The forced-swimming test (FST) is based on the observation that animals develop an immobile posture in an inescapable cylinder filled with water. In this test, immobility is interpreted as a passive stress-coping strategy or depression-like behavior (behavioral despair). After antidepressant administration, the animals will actively perform escape-directed behaviors with longer duration than animals with control saline treatment. FST is the most widely used tool in depression research, more specifically as a screen for acute antidepressants.The advantages of FST are that it is low-cost and a fast, reliable tool. It is also easy to handle and has proven its reliability across laboratories for testing potential antidepressant activities with a strong predictive validity and it allows rapid screening of large numbers of drugs. The major disadvantages of FST are that it has poor face and construct validities. The test is sensitive to acute treatment only, and its validity for non-monoamine antidepressants is uncertain

[34] The TST, also known as tail suspension test, shares a common theoretical basis and behavioral measure with the FST. In the TST, mice are suspended by their tails using adhesive tape to a horizontal bar for a certain couple of minutes, and the time of immobility is recorded. Typically, the suspended rodents perform immediately escape-like behaviors, followed by developing an immobile posture. If antidepressants are given prior to the test, the subjects will be engaged in escape-directed behaviors for longer periods of time than after saline treatment, exhibiting a decrease in duration of immobility.A major advantage of the TST is that it is simple and inexpensive. A major disadvantage of the TST is that it is restricted to mice. Like FST, TST is sensitive to acute treatment only, and its validity for non-monoamine antidepressants is uncertain.

Reward-based

Anxiety-based

[38] Rodents tend to avoid brightly illuminated areas, and this avoidance is interpreted as a symptom of anxiety. Open field is a bright enclosure and during the test rodents are placed in this arena thus forcing them to interact with a bright environment. The movement of the experimental subject will be recorded in distance and pathway.

Benefits of animal models

A benefit to this model of research is the production of antidepressants. While antidepressants are helpful, the effects of current antidepressant drugs are often significantly delayed, with improvements beginning around 3–6 weeks after treatment is started. Antidepressant screening tests provide only an end-point behavioral or physiological measure designed to assess the effect of the genetic, pharmacological, or environmental manipulation. This is unlike models which can be defined as an organism or a particular state of an organism that reproduces aspects of human pathology. Despite the clinical success of many antidepressant drugs, such as tricyclic antidepressants (TCAs), monoamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs), and serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SRIs), many individuals' symptoms are not adequately alleviated by medication alone, and other methods of treatment may be recommended.[39] [40] Antidepressant and depression research is ongoing. There is a lot more knowledge now and people struggling have access to the tools they need when seeking help. Animal research has been a successful way for experts to gain this knowledge and it continues to have positive impacts in the medical field and beyond.

See also

Notes and References

  1. Tolentino . Julio C. . Schmidt . Sergio L. . 2018-10-02 . DSM-5 Criteria and Depression Severity: Implications for Clinical Practice . Frontiers in Psychiatry . 9 . 450 . 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00450 . free . 1664-0640 . 6176119 . 30333763.
  2. Web site: Depression Basics. National Institute of Mental Health. 2013-05-07. 2013-06-11. https://web.archive.org/web/20130611091923/http://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/publications/depression/complete-index.shtml. dead.
  3. Hasler. Gregor. Drevets. Wayne C. Manji. Husseini K. Charney. Dennis S. 2004-06-23. Discovering Endophenotypes for Major Depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 29. 10. 1765–1781. 10.1038/sj.npp.1300506. 0893-133X. 15213704. free.
  4. Hao, Yuanzhen, Huixiang Ge, Mengyun Sun, and Yun Gao. 2019. "Selecting an Appropriate Animal Model of Depression" International Journal of Molecular Sciences 20, no. 19: 4827. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms20194827
  5. Chevalier . Grégoire . Siopi . Eleni . Guenin-Macé . Laure . Pascal . Maud . Laval . Thomas . Rifflet . Aline . Boneca . Ivo Gomperts . Demangel . Caroline . Colsch . Benoit . Pruvost . Alain . Chu-Van . Emeline . Messager . Aurélie . Leulier . François . Lepousez . Gabriel . Eberl . Gérard . 2020-12-11 . Effect of gut microbiota on depressive-like behaviors in mice is mediated by the endocannabinoid system . Nature Communications . en . 11 . 1 . 6363 . 10.1038/s41467-020-19931-2 . 2020NatCo..11.6363C . 2041-1723. 7732982 .
  6. Willner. P.. Mitchell. P. J.. May 2002. The validity of animal models of predisposition to depression. Behavioural Pharmacology. 13. 3. 169–188. 0955-8810. 12122308. 10.1097/00008877-200205000-00001.
  7. Anisman. Hymie. Matheson. Kim. January 2005. Stress, depression, and anhedonia: Caveats concerning animal models. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 29. 4–5. 525–546. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.007. 15925696. 5904832. 0149-7634.
  8. Vollmayr. Barbara. Mahlstedt. Magdalena M.. Henn. Fritz A.. 2007-04-01. Neurogenesis and depression: what animal models tell us about the link. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. en. 257. 5. 300–303. 10.1007/s00406-007-0734-2. 17401725. 16130112. 0940-1334.
  9. Becker . Maria . Pinhasov . Albert . Ornoy . Asher . January 14, 2021 . Animal Models of Depression: What Can They Teach Us about the Human Disease? . Diagnostics . en . 11 . 1 . 123 . 10.3390/diagnostics11010123 . free . 33466814 . 7830961 . 2075-4418.
  10. Abelaira . Helena M. . Réus . Gislaine Z. . Quevedo . João . 2013 . Animal models as tools to study the pathophysiology of depression . Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry . en . 35 . S112–S120 . 10.1590/1516-4446-2013-1098 . 1516-4446.
  11. Drugan. R. C.. Basile. A. S.. Ha. J. H.. Healy. D.. Ferland. R. J.. 1997-12-01. Analysis of the importance of controllable versus uncontrollable stress on subsequent behavioral and physiological functioning. Brain Research. Brain Research Protocols. 2. 1. 69–74. 1385-299X. 9438074. 10.1016/S1385-299X(97)00031-7. free.
  12. Grahn. R. E.. Watkins. L. R.. Maier. S. F.. July 2000. Impaired escape performance and enhanced conditioned fear in rats following exposure to an uncontrollable stressor are mediated by glutamate and nitric oxide in the dorsal raphe nucleus. Behavioural Brain Research. 112. 1–2. 33–41. 0166-4328. 10862933. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00161-3. 33316951.
  13. Durgam. Robert C.. Rodent Models of Depression: Learned Helplessness Using a Triadic Design in Rats. May 2001. Current Protocols in Neuroscience. Chapter 8. 8.10B.1–8.10B.12. 10.1002/0471142301.ns0810bs14. 978-0471142300. 18428537. 7360339.
  14. Chourbaji. S.. Zacher. C.. Sanchis-Segura. C.. Dormann. C.. Vollmayr. B.. Gass. P.. December 2005. Learned helplessness: Validity and reliability of depressive-like states in mice. Brain Research Protocols. 16. 1–3. 70–78. 10.1016/j.brainresprot.2005.09.002. 16338640. 1385-299X.
  15. Vollmayr. B.. Henn. F. A.. August 2001. Learned helplessness in the rat: improvements in validity and reliability. Brain Research. Brain Research Protocols. 8. 1. 1–7. 1385-299X. 11522522. 10.1016/S1385-299X(01)00067-8.
  16. Willner. Paul. 2005. Chronic Mild Stress (CMS) Revisited: Consistency and Behavioural-Neurobiological Concordance in the Effects of CMS. Neuropsychobiology. english. 52. 2. 90–110. 10.1159/000087097. 0302-282X. 16037678. 22504035.
  17. Willner. P.. Muscat. R.. Papp. M.. 1992. Chronic mild stress-induced anhedonia: a realistic animal model of depression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 16. 4. 525–534. 0149-7634. 1480349. 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80194-0. 9078352.
  18. Monleon. S.. D'Aquila. P.. Parra. A.. Simon. V. M.. Brain. P. F.. Willner. P.. February 1995. Attenuation of sucrose consumption in mice by chronic mild stress and its restoration by imipramine. Psychopharmacology. 117. 4. 453–457. 0033-3158. 7604147. 10.1007/BF02246218. 36727320 .
  19. Stemmelin. Jeanne. Cohen. Caroline. Yalcin. Ipek. Keane. Peter. Griebel. Guy. January 2010. Implication of β3-adrenoceptors in the antidepressant-like effects of amibegron using Adrb3 knockout mice in the chronic mild stress. Behavioural Brain Research. 206. 2. 310–312. 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.09.003. 19744528. 23269414. 0166-4328.
  20. Zhao. Dan. Xu. Xulin. Pan. Linna. Zhu. Wei. Fu. Xiaopei. Guo. Lianjun. Lu. Qing. Wang. Jian. December 2017. Pharmacologic activation of cholinergic alpha7 nicotinic receptors mitigates depressive-like behavior in a mouse model of chronic stress. Journal of Neuroinflammation. En. 14. 1. 234. 10.1186/s12974-017-1007-2. 1742-2094. 5712092. 29197398 . free .
  21. Blanchard. R. J.. McKittrick. C. R.. Blanchard. D. C.. June 2001. Animal models of social stress: effects on behavior and brain neurochemical systems. Physiology & Behavior. 73. 3. 261–271. 0031-9384. 11438351. 10.1016/S0031-9384(01)00449-8. 26571582. 2018-07-28. 2018-07-28. https://web.archive.org/web/20180728071359/http://sites.oxy.edu/clint/physio/article/animalmodelsofsocialstress.pdf. dead.
  22. Krishnan. Vaishnav. Han. Ming-Hu. Graham. Danielle L.. Berton. Olivier. Renthal. William. Russo. Scott J.. LaPlant. Quincey. Graham. Ami. Lutter. Michael. October 2007. Molecular Adaptations Underlying Susceptibility and Resistance to Social Defeat in Brain Reward Regions. Cell. 131. 2. 391–404. 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.018. 0092-8674. free. 17956738.
  23. Cryan. John F. Slattery. David A. January 2007. Animal models of mood disorders: recent developments. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 20. 1. 1–7. 10.1097/yco.0b013e3280117733. 17143074. 2320195. 0951-7367.
  24. Kudryavtseva. N. N.. Bakshtanovskaya. I. V.. Koryakina. L. A.. February 1991. Social model of depression in mice of C57BL/6J strain. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 38. 2. 315–320. 0091-3057. 2057501. 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90284-9. 24450372.
  25. McEwen. Bruce S.. 2003. Early life influences on life-long patterns of behavior and health. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews. en. 9. 3. 149–154. 10.1002/mrdd.10074. 1080-4013. 12953293.
  26. Meaney. Michael J. March 2001. Maternal Care, Gene Expression, and the Transmission of Individual Differences in Stress Reactivity Across Generations. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 24. 1. 1161–1192. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.1161. 0147-006X. 11520931.
  27. Song. Cai. Leonard. Brian E.. 2005. The olfactory bulbectomised rat as a model of depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 29. 4–5. 627–647. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.010. 15925697. 42450349. 0149-7634.
  28. O'Neil. Michael F.. Moore. Nicholas A.. 2003. Animal models of depression: are there any?. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. en. 18. 4. 239–254. 10.1002/hup.496. 12766928. 21885931. 0885-6222. free.
  29. Schramm. Nicole L.. McDonald. Michael P.. Limbird. Lee E.. 2001-07-01. The α2A-Adrenergic Receptor Plays a Protective Role in Mouse Behavioral Models of Depression and Anxiety. Journal of Neuroscience. 21. 13. 4875–4882. 10.1523/jneurosci.21-13-04875.2001. 11425914. 6762349. free.
  30. Ridder. Stephanie. Chourbaji. Sabine. Hellweg. Rainer. Urani. Alexandre. Zacher. Christiane. Schmid. Wolfgang. Zink. Mathias. Hörtnagl. Heide. Flor. Herta. 2005-06-29. Mice with Genetically Altered Glucocorticoid Receptor Expression Show Altered Sensitivity for Stress-Induced Depressive Reactions. Journal of Neuroscience. 25. 26. 6243–6250. 10.1523/jneurosci.0736-05.2005. 15987954. 6725059. free.
  31. Pliakas. A. M.. Carlson. R. R.. Neve. R. L.. Konradi. C.. Nestler. E. J.. Carlezon. W. A.. 2001-09-15. Altered responsiveness to cocaine and increased immobility in the forced swim test associated with elevated cAMP response element-binding protein expression in nucleus accumbens. The Journal of Neuroscience. 21. 18. 7397–7403. 1529-2401. 4205577. 11549750. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-18-07397.2001.
  32. Bućan. Maja. Abel. Ted. 2002. The mouse: genetics meets behaviour. Nature Reviews Genetics. En. 3. 2. 114–123. 10.1038/nrg728. 11836505. 5985686. 1471-0056.
  33. Benoit . Petit-Demouliere . Franck . Chenu . Michel . Bourin . Forced swimming test in mice: a review of antidepressant activity . Psychopharmacology . 2005 . 177 . 3 . 245–255 . 10.1007/s00213-004-2048-7 . 15609067 . 1222576 .
  34. Cryan. John F.. Mombereau. Cedric. Vassout. Annick. January 2005. The tail suspension test as a model for assessing antidepressant activity: Review of pharmacological and genetic studies in mice. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 29. 4–5. 571–625. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.009. 0149-7634. 15890404. 2758433.
  35. Nielsen. Christina Kurre. Arnt. Jorn. Sánchez. Connie. January 2000. Intracranial self-stimulation and sucrose intake differ as hedonic measures following chronic mild stress: interstrain and interindividual differences. Behavioural Brain Research. 107. 1–2. 21–33. 0166-4328. 10628727. 10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00110-2. 4001711.
  36. Verharen. Jeroen P.H.. de Jong. Johannes W.. Zhu. Yichen. Lammel. Stephan. April 2023. A computational analysis of mouse behavior in the sucrose preference test. Nature Communications. 14. 1 . 2419. 2041-1723. 37105954. 10.1038/s41467-023-38028-0. 10140068 . 2023NatCo..14.2419V .
  37. Dulawa. Stephanie C.. Hen. Rene. January 2005. Recent advances in animal models of chronic antidepressant effects: The novelty-induced hypophagia test. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 29. 4–5. 771–783. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.017. 0149-7634. 15890403. 21949826.
  38. Holmes. A.. May 2001. Targeted gene mutation approaches to the study of anxiety-like behavior in mice. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 25. 3. 261–273. 0149-7634. 11378180. 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00012-4. 7947941.
  39. Yan . Hua-Cheng . Cao . Xiong . Das . Manas . Zhu . Xin-Hong . Gao . Tian-Ming . August 2010 . Behavioral animal models of depression . Neuroscience Bulletin . en . 26 . 4 . 327–337 . 10.1007/s12264-010-0323-7 . 1673-7067 . 5552573 . 20651815.
  40. Hao, Y., Ge, H., Sun, M., & Gao, Y. (2019). Selecting an Appropriate Animal Model of Depression. International journal of molecular sciences, 20(19), 4827. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms20194827